Commitment Contracts
- Commitment contracts provide a means to counter present bias by committing a future self to an action that is difficult to evade or delay when the moment comes. One example of a commitment contract is a savings deposit that is forfeited if you fail to exert self-control over an undesired or unhealthy behavior (e.g., smoking).
- Guiding individuals to state their implementation intentions to complete a behavior has been shown to boost completion of a task and overcome present bias.
- Prompting individuals to make a plan can boost completion of a task and overcome present bias. For example, one study demonstrated that a simple prompt on a postcard to write in which vaccination clinic the employee planned to attend from the date options offered significantly increased use of the clinic in comparison with a postcard that only reminded participants of the clinic dates (see Chapter 4).
- Pairing a more desired and a less desired action together, known as temptation bundling, can counter the tendency for individuals to put off the less desired action.
- Lottery-based incentive programs leverage the fact that participants may be more motivated by the small probability of winning a large prize than by a guaranteed incentive of a much smaller prize (see Chapter 5 for examples of lottery-based incentives employed in health-related behavioral interventions).
- Foot-in-the-door techniques invite individuals to agree to a smaller request before presenting a larger one, thus resetting the reference point for the larger decision.
- The fresh start effect is the tendency to be more motivated to set or achieve goals related to key personal or cultural milestones, such as birthdays; holidays; or the start of a new year, month, or week. Sending a reminder or inviting goal setting at a fresh start moment can increase engagement. Framing (see below) a message around a fresh start can increase the intention to pursue a goal.
- Switching costs are the monetary, psychological, effort-based, and time-based costs that are incurred when individuals change services, products, brands, etc. Individuals may delay making beneficial changes when faced with switching costs; reducing or eliminating these costs can counteract this tendency to procrastinate.
- Because present bias leads people to focus on immediate costs and benefits of an action at the expense of future costs and benefits, an incentive that is delivered in the present can make benefits feel more immediate and can counter heavy weighting of immediate costs (time, hassle, etc.). Such incentives are also well-suited to incorporating other behavioral principles, including loss aversion, regret aversion, and reference dependence, into the design of interventions. For example, one study showed participants offered a $10 incentive to enroll in retirement savings increased their savings more than participants who received information about the future value of retirement savings (which far exceeded $10).
- Setting a default (e.g., automatic enrollment and salary deduction for a retirement plan [i.e., action is not required for enrollment], or a healthy default side dish on a restaurant menu) is among the most powerful strategies for behavioral change. Defaults have been shown in several systematic reviews and meta-analyses to have larger effect sizes than other intervention approaches.
- Scarcity of any kind (time, material resources, even social interaction), can make decisions about immediate tasks more costly and challenging, while also amplifying the negative consequences of poor or short-sighted decisions. Actions to mitigate scarcity factors, such as reducing the time required for a task, can improve task completion, decision quality, etc.
- Interventions that simplify the presentation of information allow people’s limited attentional resources to be directed at a smaller amount of information. For example, simplification of a two-page notice sent from the IRS to taxpayers who were eligible for the Earned Income Tax Credit but had not claimed it increased take-up of this benefit by over 10 percent.
- Reminders can counter the tendency to delay costly behaviors, especially when they mention a future goal or desired state. Reminders also cut through inattention, remedy prospective memory failures (i.e., forgetting), and increase salience of the desired behavior.
- The importance of social relationships can also be invoked by interventions that point out the beneficial effects that an individual’s choices can have on others. Some laboratory experiments have successfully used altruism appeals to increase prosocial behavioral intentions (e.g., to get vaccinated).
- People’s mental representations of how the world works can strongly influence decisions and behaviors in diverse domains, including health, climate change, and management. Techniques to revise or update mental models include visualization, experiencing surprise, analogies, simulation, and critical reflection (see Chapter 4 for references).
- Social proof interventions provide information about other people’s choices and behaviors, such as the number of drinks most students consume at a party or how many other voters plan to vote or have already voted. Such interventions have been used to reduce risky or harmful behaviors, such as binge drinking, and to increase positive behaviors, such as voter engagement.
- Social comparison interventions explicitly compare an individual’s performance with a relevant comparison or reference group. (See Chapter 4 for discussion of evidence about when this strategy is effective, and when it can create backfire effects.)
- Present bias leads individuals to delay action when they experience friction, hassle factors, or administrative burden. Reducing or removing sources of friction, hassle factors, and administrative burden can counteract people’s tendency to procrastinate by reducing the immediate costs (in the form of time or inconvenience) of a behavior. Examples of such interventions include providing individualized information about a benefit so that people do not have to seek that information themselves, and allowing users to complete an action online rather than by mail or in person.
- When inattention and cognitive load create barriers to behavior change, it can be an effective strategy to make crucial information or features of a choice set more salient (i.e., calling attention to particular information, such as grocery store price tags noting the tax rate applied to items, resulting in a decrease in the purchase of taxable items; see Chapter 3). Salience prime interventions have been shown to be effective in many domains.
- Choice Sets - Limited attention for choices can be focused through the creation, arrangement, and framing through choice sets. Reducing the number of choices available has been shown to improve the selection process and increase people’s satisfaction with their choices. How options are described and displayed can influence the choice an individual makes. Choice sets can be designed to reduce comparison friction – the cognitive burden caused by having to compare choices across multiple attributes without sufficient information.
- Active Choice - Active choice interventions create a stopping point in a process that requires an individual to make a choice before proceeding (e.g., being asked to choose whether or not to enroll in automatic prescription refill when calling a pharmacy customer service line). Active choice is useful for focusing limited attention when it is not possible (or perceived as not ethical) to implement a true default.
- The way a choice is framed can significantly influence the outcome of the decision. This phenomenon, known as the framing effect, has been studied in a variety of settings. A common framing strategy for behavioral interventions is a loss frame, which leverages loss aversion by drawing attention to the negative consequences of inaction. Robust evidence shows that loss-frame messages are effective for screening or disease detection behaviors, while gain-frame messages are more effective for prevention behaviors and prosocial behaviors.
- Social influence, also known as social modeling, involves having celebrities or respected opinion leaders endorse or model a desired behavior, such as recycling.
- People tend to display reciprocity—responding to being treated generously by behaving more generously themselves and vice versa if they are treated unkindly. For example, one study showed that a reciprocity prime (the statement “I would accept an organ from a deceased donor in order to save my own life”) resulted in increased organ donor intentions.
- Creating slack involves providing more space or flexibility for people’s responses, such as introducing buffers of extra time or extra financial resources and reducing the cost of errors.
- De-biasing can be employed to correct mitigate the effects of inaccurate beliefs and biases. For example, presenting statistics related to past performance of either an individual or a relevant reference group can counteract overoptimism bias – the tendency for a person to believe they will perform better than average or that good things are more likely to come their way.
Examples of strategies that have been shown to be effective:
- Defaults
a default salary deduction for a retirement plan, or a healthy default side dish on a restaurant menu
- Simplification or removing of hassle factors
allowing users to complete an action online instead of by mail or in person
- Immediacy
offering an incentive for an action taken that is delivered immediately
- Framing of choice sets
designing choice sets to reduce the cognitive burden caused by having to compare choices without enough information
For a more comprehensive list of intervention strategies with explanations, please see Chapter 4
Applying Behavioral Economics to Public Policy
Intervention strategies appear to show effectiveness when they are tailored to specific circumstances and populations, especially in the six public policy domains chosen for this report. These domains represent areas of policy importance in which behavioral economics principles have been applied.
- Health
- Retirement Savings
- Social Safety Net Benefits
- Climate Change
- Education
- Criminal Justice
Modest, low-cost interventions that target very specific challenges have the most value, along with the cumulative value of small-scale, low-cost interventions. Behaviorally based interventions can lower barriers to medication adherence; promote health practices like colorectal cancer screening, HIV prevention and treatment adherence, and vaccination; and strengthen providers’ adherence to practice guidelines. Chapter 5: Health
Substantial research has shown the effectiveness of making retirement savings a default choice for employees; evidence from this line of research has been strong enough to influence federal legislation. Chapter 6: Retirement Benefits
Evidence in this domain is mixed. Some interventions show modest effects on take-up, with others showing no significant effect (or any effect). However, a few more costly large-scale interventions, like those implemented for the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP), have shown positive effects on take-up by simplifying application forms and reducing the administrative burden. Chapter 7: Social Safety Net Benefits
With climate change, there evidence demonstrating high value in targeting specific concerns and the cumulative value of multiple small-scale, low-cost interventions. Providing consumers information that addresses their present bias, limited attention and cognition, and uses nudges related to social norms can encourage energy conservation, climate-friendly transportation decisions, and engagement in land use conservation programs. Chapter 8: Climate Change
Interventions here have reduced the administrative burdens of applications; provided support to students; and set up default choices, like automatic registration for college admissions tests, leading to increases in college applications and applications for financial aid. Chapter 9: Education
Behavioral factors influence all points of the criminal justice system: determinants of criminal behavior, policing practices, court proceedings, judicial decision making, and incarceration. Evidence for the effects of behavioral interventions at different steps of the criminal justice system are too scant to provide conclusive evidence on their effectiveness. More research is needed on the role of behavioral factors on the criminal justice system. Chapter 10: Criminal Justice System
Learn More
- Read report
- Download report PDF
- View additional resources
- Learn more about the study